السبت، 18 فبراير 2012

Features of Women's Language

By a close analysis of women's language, Lakoff has found that there are certain features that characterize the speech of women and are not found in the speech of men. The suggested features could be gathered under 'hedging devices' and 'boasting devices'. As the name suggests, the 'hedging devices' are used to show confusion and uncertainty. The 'boasting devices' are used to strengthen the meaning. To exemplify this, it was a bad day can express certainty by saying it was really a bad day. It can also show uncertainty by saying: it was a kind of a bad day. Lakoff argued that both kinds show the unconfidence of the females. They only use the 'boasting devices' to convince their addressee with their utterance (Holmes 310).

a.     Lexical Choice
     Lakoff noticed that there are certain words uttered only by women. These words may be related to color such as 'mauve' and 'chartreuse'. She says that women also use adjectives which are out of meaning and power as 'divine' and 'cute'. This is contrasted to the adjectives used by men such as 'great' and 'terrific'. However, we should pay attention to the fact that this study is based on introspection and not on introspection and not on empirical methods (Wolfson 176).

b.     Question intonation in statements
     That feature can be categorized under the 'hedging devices'. Lakoff argues that women show their uncertainty by intonating questions in declarative sentences. In other words, they make questions and suggestions when they are asked about something by their addressee (Wolfson 176). For example, when they are asked about the time when the dinner would be ready, they are going to say "oh, about eight O'clock?"(Lakoff qtd in Wolfson 176). In that case, they have changed their answer into a question because they wanted to escape authority, or that they wanted only to make it that way.

c.     Hedging
      In addition to intonating questions in declarative statements, women would hedge their speech acts to avoid being direct and to the point. They often do this by adding certain fillers such as; you know, sort of, you see. They might also do this by adding a tag question to their locutionary acts. For example, they might say: the window is open, isn't it? What they meant by this is to get the window closed by the hearer, but they tagged their question to avoid a direct request. In that respect, they use certain modifiers that show uncertainty such as kind of and you know what I mean. This is because they want to get an approval from the other participant. However, tag questions may be used to express anger or threat. For example, if a woman says "so you think you can get away with that, do you?" she does not mean avoiding being direct, but rather expressing her anger or threat (Wolfson 177).

d.     Emphatic Modifiers and Intonational Emphasis
     "[W]omen use the modifiers so, such, and very to emphasize their utterances much more frequently than men do and that  they combine this usage with an intensity of intonation out of proportion with the topic of the phrase" (Wolfson 177). As said before, they tend to emphasize their utterances because they feel from their inside that the addressee is not believing them. Thus, they use such 'boasting devices' which in reality show their uncertainty. As an exemplification of this, if you asked a woman about the performance of an actor, she would say "it was a BRILLIANT performance" (Holmes 310).

e.     Hypercorrect grammar and pronunciation
     That is a consistent use of standard verb forms. Women use more prestigious words than men do in the same situations. This is applicable also to the pronunciation and meaning. Labov and Trudgill discovered that women of the 'lower-middle class' are more likely to use words nearer to the 'prestige norm', because women (in the period of these experiments) were isolated. Hence, they are nearer to hypercorrection (Wolfson 177).

f. Redundancy
     The speech of women is full of redundancy. They tend to repeat what they have just said. On the other side, men are more likely to omit 'non-essential' utterances more than women. For example:
A: Male:" I'm employed with…. Aah been there over nine months".
B: Female: "My name is Sophia…. I've been employed…"
In those two examples, it is clear that the male prefers to omit what would be understood by his hearer. On the contrary, Sophia repeats every single detail in her utterances. She uses the complete form of "I've been" (Holmes 319). The funniest thing is that it may be said, as a result of the existence of this feature in the utterance of women, that women are more talkative than men. However, recent surveys have proved that men are more talkative than women.

g.     Interactions and Interruptions
     Although it is known that women are more talkative than men, most of the recent researches show that this is not true (Holmes 320). Men are more likely to interrupt others while they are speaking with them. Most studies prove the fact that while interacting with females, males are more likely to interrupt them:
  Wanda: Did you see here that sociologists have just proved that men interrupt
women all the time? They____
  Ralph: Who says?
  Wanda: … They think it's a dominance trick men aren't even aware of. But ____
  Ralph: These people have nothing better to do than eavesdrop on interruptions?
  Wanda: -but women make 'retrievals' about one third of the time…they pick up where
they left off after the man___
   Ralph: Surely not all men are like that Wanda?
   Wanda: … Doesn't that___
   Ralph: speaking as… (Holmes 321).
In that conversation, it is apparent that Ralph is interrupting Wanda all the time. She cannot even utter a complete sentence without being interrupted by Ralph. Females are even more interrupted more than men whether she is doctor, wife, patient, daughter, or student (Holmes 322).

h.     Feedback
     Women tend to provide feedbacks (like mmm) to the utterance of the other participants more than men. Another study shows that females tend to develop and widen the arguments of the other speakers. It is deduced that females are more 'cooperative conversationalists' than men. On the other hand, men tend not to support but to compete with the other participant's arguments (Holmes 324).

i. Overlap
     Gender differences in a certain language do not reflect the use of completely different forms between men and women. They only overlap the similar forms. For instance, while women pronounce the –ing in a word like catching as [ing], men pronounce it as [in]. In Sydney, men utter the first sound in (thing) as [f] more than women. Men also do not pronounce 'h's as in 'ome and 'ouse. Such examples show that women use a hypercorrect language more than men do. On the contrary, men tend to use a vulgar language than woman (Holmes 176).

j. Gossips through Women's Language
     The gossip of females has certain linguistic features. It contains intensifiers that show certainty. It also contains question tags that require the interference of the other participants. When all the participants are women, they complete their sayings. In other words, it expresses the "cooperative and positive nature" of the females talk (Holmes 327).

4.     Gender- Related Directives
     Females are more likely to use more polite forms of directives than men and boys. A study of how doctors use directives can clarify this. While a male doctor may tell his patient, 'lie down', a female doctor may address her patient saying: "maybe you could stay away from …." Directives delivered to women are also more likely to be in a polite form, and even less direct than that are delivered to men (Holmes 193&194). An example of this would be:
(a)  Tom: Give me that. I need it now.
(b)  Seymour: Get off that car.
(c)   Grant: Get out of my house.
(d)  Maria: You finished with that rolling pin now.
(e)   Lisa: My turn now eh?
(f)    Meg: It's time for tea so you'll have to go home now (Holmes 193).
  

ليست هناك تعليقات:

إرسال تعليق